Reinforcement reduces the size–latency phenomenon: A cost–benefit evaluation of saccade triggering

Article


Vullings, C., Harwood, M. and Madelain, L. 2019. Reinforcement reduces the size–latency phenomenon: A cost–benefit evaluation of saccade triggering. Journal of Vision. 19 (4), p. Art. 16. https://doi.org/10.1167/19.4.16
AuthorsVullings, C., Harwood, M. and Madelain, L.
Abstract

Saccadic latencies are known to change as a function of target eccentricity and size. Recently, it has been shown that latencies consistently change according to the amplitude of the step in proportion to the size of the target (Madelain, Krauzlis, & Wallman, 2005; Harwood, Madelain, Krauzlis, & Wallman, 2008; De Vries, Azadi, & Harwood, 2016). This effect, called the size–latency phenomenon, might be seen as a function of a cost–benefit relationship: Longer latencies might be explained by the lower benefit of making a saccade while the target mostly remains within the attentional field. Here, we probe this hypothesis by manipulating the cost–benefit relationship using a reinforcement procedure. Participants tracked a target stepping horizontally with varying amplitudes and sizes such that the step-to-size ratio was equal to either 0.3 or 1.5. We used a dynamic-reinforcement criterion in the blocked conditions. In the 0.3-ratio condition, any latency shorter than the criterion was reinforced. In the 1.5-ratio condition, any latency longer than the criterion was reinforced. During baseline, we observed the size–latency effect with large differences in latencies depending on the ratio in force (229 and 161 ms, respectively, for 0.3 and 1.5). After learning, distributions shifted toward the shorter or longer value (198 and 236 ms, respectively, for 0.3 and 1.5). On average, latencies decreased by 31 ms and increased by 75 ms according to the ongoing reinforcement contingencies. Our results indicate that reinforcement contingencies can considerably affect saccadic-latency distributions, and support the idea of a cost–benefit evaluation of saccade triggering.

JournalJournal of Vision
Journal citation19 (4), p. Art. 16
ISSN1534-7362
Year2019
PublisherAssociation for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology (ARVO)
Publisher's version
License
File Access Level
Anyone
Digital Object Identifier (DOI)https://doi.org/10.1167/19.4.16
Publication dates
Print10 Apr 2019
Publication process dates
Accepted08 Jan 2019
Deposited26 Apr 2019
FunderAgence Nationale de Recherches
National Science Foundation
Agence Nationale de Recherches
National Science Foundation
Copyright holder© 2019 The Authors
Permalink -

https://repository.uel.ac.uk/item/843zz

Download files


Publisher's version
i1534-7362-19-4-16.pdf
License: CC BY-NC-ND 4.0
File access level: Anyone

  • 169
    total views
  • 169
    total downloads
  • 0
    views this month
  • 0
    downloads this month

Export as

Related outputs

Adaptation of naturally paced saccades
Gray, Michael J., Blangero, Annabelle, Herman, James P., Wallman, Josh and Harwood, M. 2014. Adaptation of naturally paced saccades. Journal of Neurophysiology. 111 (11), pp. 2343-2354. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00905.2013
Visual cues that are effective for contextual saccade adaptation
Azadi, Reza and Harwood, M. 2014. Visual cues that are effective for contextual saccade adaptation. Journal of Neurophysiology. 111 (11), pp. 2307-2319. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00894.2013
Salient Distractors Can Induce Saccade Adaptation
Khan, Afsheen, McFadden, Sally A., Harwood, M. and Wallman, Josh 2014. Salient Distractors Can Induce Saccade Adaptation. Journal of Ophthalmology. 2014 (585792), pp. 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/585792
The saccadic size-latency phenomenon explored: Proximal target size is a determining factor in the saccade latency
De Vries, J.P., Azadi, R. and Harwood, M. 2016. The saccadic size-latency phenomenon explored: Proximal target size is a determining factor in the saccade latency. Vision Research. 129, pp. 87-97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2016.09.006