An exploratory study of Forest School practitioners’ views on the benefits of and barriers to children’s participation in Forest School

Conference item


Hepworth, A. and Edmonds, C. 2025. An exploratory study of Forest School practitioners’ views on the benefits of and barriers to children’s participation in Forest School. First International Forest School Symposium. Forest School Association.
AuthorsHepworth, A. and Edmonds, C.
Abstract

A unique feature of Forest School is the role of the Forest School leader. In the UK, Forest School practitioners are highly trained in both theory and practice via routes accredited by the UK Forest School Association, according to their six good practice principles and pedagogy (Forest School Association, 2011; Knight, 2011). Forest School practitioners facilitate child-centred and child-initiated learning (Cree, 2009) and provide an important learning space that is different to the traditional indoor classroom (Harris, 2018; Waite & Goodenough, 2018). In the UK, primary school aged children who attend Forest School, rarely do so full time, but instead attend sessions as part of their mainstream schooling and Forest School leaders may be independent of the school, or may be a trained teacher or teaching assistant (Waite & Goodenough, 2018).

Regular participation in Forest School benefits children in many different ways, as outlined by two recent systematic reviews (Dabaja, 2022; Garden & Downes, 2021). Dabaja (2022) focused on educational and other impacts and reported seven areas in which children were positively impacted by participation in Forest School. These include social and cooperative skills, physical skills, self-confidence and self- esteem, learning performance and cognitive skills, emotional and mental wellbeing, environmental awareness and sense of belonging, and risk management skills. Garden and Downes (2021) focused solely on Forest School and education and grouped the benefits of Forest School by age group and whether children had special educational needs and Disability (SEND). For children with SEND, areas improved by Forest School participation included risk and relationships. For those in the Early Years (under 5 years), benefits were found in natural and risky play and development. For those in Formal education (5 years to 18 years), physical activity, mental health and environment awareness all benefitted from Forest School participation. The benefits identified in these reviews overlap and we chose a subset to examine in the present study.

Research has also identified a number of barriers to children’s participation in Forest School. These include an already busy curriculum, inflexible timetabling, and pressures related to pedagogical ideology (Bentsen, Jensen, Mygind, & Randrup, 2010; Waite, Bølling, & Bentsen, 2016). Some schools may have an appropriate green space on their site for Forest School, but many will need to travel, and the costs and time associated with this may be prohibitive, or may limit the number of children and/or sessions that are possible (Bentsen et al., 2010). Costs associated with appropriate outdoor equipment, including clothing for inclement weather, may also be problematic (Bentsen et al., 2010). Some schools may experience, or perceive, negativity from parents, who may not recognise the benefit of Forest School for their children (Waite et al., 2016).

Much of the evidence on the benefits of participating in Forest School, and the barriers to participation, comes from studies that interview practitioners and use qualitative methods of analysis (Dabaja, 2022; Garden & Downes, 2021). As quantitative researchers, we trialled the use of rating scales, which if successful, could subsequently be used on a larger sample. Alongside this, we asked participants for additional written comments. In the present, exploratory, study, Forest School practitioners were asked to rate on Likert scales the extent to which they felt that participating in Forest School benefited children’s learning through play, self-esteem, nature connection, concentration, language, and cooperative skills and the extent to which they felt the following barriers were an issue for participation: poor weather, curriculum demands, lack of outdoor space and parents’ expectations.

Year2025
ConferenceFirst International Forest School Symposium
PublisherForest School Association
Publisher's version
Publication dates
Print30 May 2025
Publication process dates
Deposited23 Jun 2025
Journal citationpp. 70-78
Book titleFirst International Forest School Symposium: Extended Abstracts
Book editorKnight, S.
ISBN978-1-0369-2090-6
Web address (URL) of conference proceedingshttps://forestschoolassociation.org/forest-school-research-symposium-proceedings/#!form/SymposiumProceedings
Copyright holder© 2025 The Authors
Permalink -

https://repository.uel.ac.uk/item/8zqzw

Download files

  • 18
    total views
  • 5
    total downloads
  • 18
    views this month
  • 5
    downloads this month

Export as

Related outputs

Co-occurring Impairments in several domains of Memory following Neonatal Hypoxic-Ischaemic Encephalopathy have Real-life Implications
Holder, A., Cianfaglione, R., Burns, J., Vollmer, B. and Edmonds, C.J. 2025. Co-occurring Impairments in several domains of Memory following Neonatal Hypoxic-Ischaemic Encephalopathy have Real-life Implications. European Journal of Paediatric Neurology. 55, pp. 9-17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2025.03.002
Attending 12 weekly sessions of Forest School sessions improves mood and cooperation in 7-8 year old children
Hepworth, A., Haddad, H. and Edmonds, C. 2024. Attending 12 weekly sessions of Forest School sessions improves mood and cooperation in 7-8 year old children. Journal of Adventure Education and Outdoor Learning. In Press. https://doi.org/10.1080/14729679.2024.2393130
Water Consumption Increases Handwriting Speed and Volume Consumed Relates to Increased Finger‑tapping Speed in Schoolchildren
Booth, P., Hunyadvari, N., Dawkins, L., Moore, D., Gentile‑Rapinett, G. and Edmonds, C. J. 2022. Water Consumption Increases Handwriting Speed and Volume Consumed Relates to Increased Finger‑tapping Speed in Schoolchildren. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement. 6, pp. 183-191. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-021-00232-5
Neonatal Hypoxic-Ischaemic Encephalopathy: Motor Impairment beyond Cerebral Palsy
Erdi-Krausz, G., Rocha, R., Brown, A., Myneni, A., Lennartsson, F., Romsauerova, A., Cianfaglione, R., Edmonds, C. and Vollmer, B. 2021. Neonatal Hypoxic-Ischaemic Encephalopathy: Motor Impairment beyond Cerebral Palsy. European Journal of Paediatric Neurology. 35, pp. 74-81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2021.10.005
Drinking Water Enhances Cognitive Performance: Positive Effects on Working Memory but Not Long-Term Memory
Edmonds, C., Beeley, J., Rizzo, I., Booth, P. and Gardner, M. 2021. Drinking Water Enhances Cognitive Performance: Positive Effects on Working Memory but Not Long-Term Memory. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement. 6, p. 67–73. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-021-00225-4
Children with neonatal Hypoxic Ischemic Encephalopathy (HIE) treated with therapeutic hypothermia are not as school ready as their peers
Edmonds, C. J., Cianfaglione, R., Cornforth, C. and Vollmer, B. 2021. Children with neonatal Hypoxic Ischemic Encephalopathy (HIE) treated with therapeutic hypothermia are not as school ready as their peers. Acta Paediatrica. 110 (10), pp. 2756-2765. https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.16002
Dehydration in older people: a systematic review of the effects of dehydration on health outcomes, healthcare costs and cognitive performance
Edmonds, C., Foglia, E., Booth, P., Fu, C. and Gardner, M. 2021. Dehydration in older people: a systematic review of the effects of dehydration on health outcomes, healthcare costs and cognitive performance. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics. 95 (Art. 104380). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2021.104380
Taking Class Notes by Hand Compared to Typing: Effects on Children’s Recall and Understanding
Horbury, S. R. and Edmonds, C. 2020. Taking Class Notes by Hand Compared to Typing: Effects on Children’s Recall and Understanding. Journal of Research in Childhood Education. 35 (1), pp. 55-67. https://doi.org/10.1080/02568543.2020.1781307
Minor neurological signs and behavioural function at age 2 years in neonatal hypoxic ischaemic encephalopathy (HIE)
Edmonds, C., Helps, A., Hart, D., Zatorska, A. M., Gupta, N., Cianfaglione, R. and Vollmer, B. 2020. Minor neurological signs and behavioural function at age 2 years in neonatal hypoxic ischaemic encephalopathy (HIE). European Journal of Paediatric Neurology. 27, pp. 78-85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2020.04.003
The impact of water consumption on hydration and cognition among schoolchildren: Methods and results from a crossover trial in rural Mali
Chard, A, Trinies, V, Edmonds, C., Soggore, A and Freeman, MC 2019. The impact of water consumption on hydration and cognition among schoolchildren: Methods and results from a crossover trial in rural Mali. PLoS ONE. 14 (1), p. e0210568. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210568
At what stage in the drinking process does drinking water affect attention and memory? Effects of mouth rinsing and mouth drying in adults
Edmonds, C., Skeete, J., Klamerus, E. and Gardner, M. 2019. At what stage in the drinking process does drinking water affect attention and memory? Effects of mouth rinsing and mouth drying in adults. Psychological Research. 85, p. 214–222. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01229-8
School Age Neurological and Cognitive Outcomes of Fetal Growth Retardation or Small for Gestational Age Birth Weight
Vollmer, Brigitte and Edmonds, C. 2019. School Age Neurological and Cognitive Outcomes of Fetal Growth Retardation or Small for Gestational Age Birth Weight. Frontiers in Endocrinology. 10, p. Art. 186. https://doi.org/10.3389/fendo.2019.00186
How does drinking water affect attention and memory? The effect of mouth rinsing and mouth drying on children's performance
Edmonds, C., Harte, Naomi and Gardner, Mark 2018. How does drinking water affect attention and memory? The effect of mouth rinsing and mouth drying on children's performance. Physiology & Behavior. 194, pp. 233-238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.06.004
Implicit Mentalising during Level-1 Visual Perspective-Taking Indicated by Dissociation with Attention Orienting
Gardner, M.R., Beleviciute, A.P. and Edmonds, C. 2018. Implicit Mentalising during Level-1 Visual Perspective-Taking Indicated by Dissociation with Attention Orienting. Vision. 2 (3). https://doi.org/10.3390/vision2010003
'Spontaneous' visual perspective-taking mediated by attention orienting that is voluntary and not reflexive
Gardner, Mark R., Hull, Zainabb, Taylor, Donna and Edmonds, C. 2018. 'Spontaneous' visual perspective-taking mediated by attention orienting that is voluntary and not reflexive. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 71 (4), pp. 1020-1029. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2017.1307868
Dose-Response Effects of Water Supplementation on Cognitive Performance and Mood in Children and Adults
Edmonds, C., Crosbie, Laura, Fatima, Fareeha, Hussain, Maryam, Jacob, Nicole and Gardner, Mark 2016. Dose-Response Effects of Water Supplementation on Cognitive Performance and Mood in Children and Adults. Appetite. 108, pp. 464-470. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2016.11.011
The effect of carbohydrate mouth rinsing on fencing performance and cognitive function following fatigue-inducing fencing
Rowlatt, G., Bottoms, Lindsay, Edmonds, C. and Buscombe, R. 2016. The effect of carbohydrate mouth rinsing on fencing performance and cognitive function following fatigue-inducing fencing. European Journal Of Sport Science. 17 (4), pp. 433-440. https://doi.org/10.1080/17461391.2016.1251497
Observed bodies generate object-based spatial codes
Taylor, Alison, Flynn, Maria, Edmonds, C. and Gardner, Mark R. 2016. Observed bodies generate object-based spatial codes. Acta Psychologica. 169 (Sep.), pp. 71-78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.05.009
The effect of carbohydrate mouth rinsing on fencing performance and cognitive function following a fatigue inducing simulated bout of fencing in national level foil fencers
Bottoms, L., Rowlatt, Georgina, Edmonds, C. and Buscombe, R. 2016. The effect of carbohydrate mouth rinsing on fencing performance and cognitive function following a fatigue inducing simulated bout of fencing in national level foil fencers. Biomedical Basis of Elite Performance 2016. Nottingham, UK 06 - 08 Mar 2016
The effect of carbohydrate mouth rinse on a 30-minute arm cranking performance
Andersson, H., Sinclair, J., Knight, A., Buscombe, R., Edmonds, C. and Bottoms, L. 2016. The effect of carbohydrate mouth rinse on a 30-minute arm cranking performance. Comparative Exercise Physiology. 12 (1), pp. 41-47. https://doi.org/10.3920/CEP150032
Subjective thirst moderates changes in speed of responding associated with water consumption
Edmonds, C., Crombie, R. and Gardner, M. R. 2013. Subjective thirst moderates changes in speed of responding associated with water consumption. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 7, p. Art363. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00363
Strategy modulates spatial perspective-taking: evidence for dissociable disembodied and embodied routes
Gardner, Mark R, Brazier, Mark, Edmonds, C. and Gronholm, Petra C. 2013. Strategy modulates spatial perspective-taking: evidence for dissociable disembodied and embodied routes. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 7 (Art. 457). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00457
Water consumption, not expectancies about water consumption, affects cognitive performance in adults
Edmonds, C., Crombie, Rosanna, Baillieux, Haiko, Gardner, Mark R and Dawkins, L. 2013. Water consumption, not expectancies about water consumption, affects cognitive performance in adults. Appetite. 60, pp. 148-153.
Expectation of having consumed caffeine can improve performance and mood
Dawkins, L., Shahzad, Fatima-Zahra, Ahmed, Suada S. and Edmonds, C. 2011. Expectation of having consumed caffeine can improve performance and mood. Appetite. 57 (3), pp. 597-600.
The effect of intra-uterine growth on Verbal IQ scores in childhood: a monozygotic twin study
Edmonds, C., Isaacs, Elizabeth B., Cole, Tim J., Haslinger Rogers, Mary, Lanigan, Julie, Singhal, Atul, Birbara, Toni, Gringras, Paul and Denton, Jane 2010. The effect of intra-uterine growth on Verbal IQ scores in childhood: a monozygotic twin study. Pediatrics. 126 (5), pp. e1095-e1101.
Generating inferences from written and spoken language: a comparison of children with visual impairment and children with sight
Edmonds, C. and Pring, Linda 2006. Generating inferences from written and spoken language: a comparison of children with visual impairment and children with sight. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 24 (2), pp. 337-351.
Inspection time and cognitive abilities in twins aged 7 to 17 years: age-related changes, heritability and genetic covariance
Edmonds, C., Isaacs, Elizabeth B., Visscher, Peter M., Rogers, Mary, Lanigan, Julie, Singhal, Atul, Lucas, Alan, Gringras, Paul, Denton, Jane and Deary, Ian J. 2008. Inspection time and cognitive abilities in twins aged 7 to 17 years: age-related changes, heritability and genetic covariance. Intelligence. 36 (3), pp. 210-225.
Should children drink more water? The effects of drinking water on cognition in children
Edmonds, C. and Burford, Denise 2009. Should children drink more water? The effects of drinking water on cognition in children. Appetite. 52 (3), pp. 776-779.
Aluminum exposure from parenteral nutrition in preterm infants: bone health at 15-year follow-up
Fewtrell, Mary S., Bishop, Nick J., Edmonds, C., Isaacs, Elizabeth B. and Lucas, Alan 2009. Aluminum exposure from parenteral nutrition in preterm infants: bone health at 15-year follow-up. Pediatrics. 124 (5), pp. 1372-1379.
Does having a drink help you think? 6–7-year-old children show improvements in cognitive performance from baseline to test after having a drink of water
Edmonds, C. and Jeffes, Ben 2009. Does having a drink help you think? 6–7-year-old children show improvements in cognitive performance from baseline to test after having a drink of water. Appetite. 53 (3), pp. 469-472.